|
|
|
Clustering and Allophones |
|
|
|
|
1. There are two kinds of linking, one is a
vowel links another vowel and the other, a consonant links another vowel or
consonant. We will start with the linking between vowels. In this part,
there are two glides that serve to link the high vowels with other vowels.
Glide /y/ is used to link between /i/ and other vowels. For instance, |
|
|
|
879 /i/ reality [riÈJQlDtö] be active [biÈJQktIv] |
|
/e/ playoff [ÈpleJDf] pay
off [ÈpeJDf] |
|
/AI/ triangle [trAIÈJQNgl`] my ankle [mAIÈJQNkl`] |
|
/I/ annoyance [DÈnIJDns] |
|
The boy and me [DDÈbIJDnmi] |
|
|
|
|
2. And glide /w/ is used to link between /u/ and
other vowels. For instance |
|
|
|
880 /u/ fluid [ÈfluWId] do it [duWIt] |
|
/o/ snowy [snoWö] go out [goÈWAUt] |
|
/AU/ flour [flAUWÔ] how is it [ÈhAUWIzIt] |
|
/ju/ newest [ÈnuWIst] |
|
a few apples [«fjuÈWQplÁz] |
|
|
|
|
|
|
1. I can’t study with the radio on. |
|
2. He is a creative piano player. |
|
3. The biologist has a lot of experience in this
area. |
|
4. No one wants to go out with me. |
|
5. Who ate the apples? |
|
6. There’re still two apples left. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
3. As for the non-high vowels, there is no glide
to serve as a link between two vowels. Some dialects in English may use /// and /r/ to link these
vowels. For example, |
|
|
|
883 |
|
1. /«/ banana oil [b«ÈnQn«/ÇIl] |
|
2. // saw us [Èsr«s] |
|
|
|
|
The second kind of linking is between the
consonants and the vowel right after it. It is often understood as the
consonant that ends the first word being pronounced intervocalically as if
it belonged to two syllables. However, the data show that the ending
consonant of the first word is completely different from a starting
consonant of another word. Compare the following sounds: |
|
|
|
884 1. gray tissue |
|
great issue |
|
2. I scream |
|
ice cream |
|
|
|
|
When a word ending in a single consonant is
followed by a word beginning with a vowel, the final consonant of the word
is often pronounced as part of the following syllable. |
|
|
|
885 1. Look at me. |
|
2. Pick it up. |
|
3. I couldn’t figure it out. |
|
4. Not at all. |
|
5. What are you doing? |
|
|
|
|
When a word ending in a consonant cluster is
followed by a word beginning with a vowel, the final consonant of the
cluster is often pronounced as part of the following syllable (resyllabification). |
|
|
|
887 1. I won’t accept it. |
|
2. He looked at me. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
When a word ending in a consonant is followed by
a word beginning with the same consonant, the two consonants will be
pronounced as one single consonant. |
|
|
|
889 1. a big girl |
|
2. walks slowly |
|
3. stop peeping |
|
4. real life |
|
5. a hot topic |
|
|
|
|
When a word ending in a consonant is followed by
a word beginning with another consonant, the two consonants will be
pronounced as two consonants. If the ending consonant is a stop consonant,
the ending stop consonant is not released before the following consonant is
pronounced (especially when it is followed by another stop or affricate
consonant). |
|
|
|
891 1. Kiss me. |
|
2. It’s a great book. |
|
3. He’s a good cook. |
|
4. Kids love Big Bird. |
|
|
|
|
Assimilation means that one sound is altered to
become more like its neighbor. For example, in “sit down,” the ending /t/
is assimilated into the following /d/ sound. The /T/ in “worth”
becomes /D/ in “worthy.” There are four types assimilation in English: |
|
|
|
1. progressive / forward / perseverative
assimilation |
|
2. regressive / anticipatory assimilation |
|
3. full / complete assimilation |
|
4. coalescent assimilation |
|
|
|
|
Progressive assimilation refers to one sound
preceding and affecting the following sound. The most common types of these
sounds are the inflections of conjugation and plural forms. The “-s” suffix
following the voiceless consonants is always pronounced as /s/ and always
pronounced as /z/ if following a vowel or a voiced consonant. For example: |
|
893 1. look looks |
|
2. love loves |
|
3. lie lies |
|
|
|
4. lie lied |
|
5. look looked |
|
6. love loved |
|
|
|
|
Regressive assimilation refers to one sound
preceding but being affected the following sound. For example: |
|
|
|
894 |
|
1. “input” is pronounced like [ÈImpUt] |
|
2. “pancake” is pronounced like [ÈpQNkek] |
|
3. “have to” is pronounced like [ÈhQft«] |
|
4. “has to” is pronounced like [ÈhQst«] |
|
5. “used to” |
|
|
|
|
Full or complete assimilation refers to one
sound being changed in the direction of a neighboring sound. For example: |
|
|
|
895 |
|
1. “cupboard” is pronounced like [ÈkÃbÔd],
not [ÈkÃpbÔd] |
|
2. “horseshoe” is pronounced like [ÈhrSu]
rather than [ÈhrsSu] |
|
|
|
|
Coalescent assimilation refers to two adjacent
sounds producing a new sound that is between the two original sounds. (alveolar
+ palatal palato-alveolar) |
|
896 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
897 |
|
1.
sure; pressure issue social |
|
2. leisure; pleasure |
|
3. statue graduate |
|
4. perpetual individual |
|
5. nature procedure |
|
6. question vacation essential |
|
7. righteous |
|
8. actual virtue gesture |
|
9. graduate modular |
|
10. expression mission |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Compared with assimilation in English,
dissimilation is less common. Dissimilation means the changes which occur
when one of two recurring sounds is altered to become less like its
neighbor (p. 215, Bronstein). In these instances, either one of the two
repeated sounds is dropped or it is changed into a different sound. For
example, in “governor” the first /r/ is dropped and pronounced as /ÈgÃvn1Ô/
without pronouncing the first /r/ sound. According to Celce-Murcia et al.
(1996), “In English, the process of dissimilation can be ignored for
pedagogical purpose” (p. 162). However, they still listed seven categories
for instances of deletion (p. 162). |
|
|
|
|
Loss of /t/ when /nt/ is between a stressed and
unstressed syllables: e.g., winter, Toronto, enter, mental |
|
|
|
Examples: |
|
898 1.
Winter is too cold here. |
|
2.
Toronto is a large city. |
|
3.
He entered the room. |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Loss of /t/ or /d/ when they occur second in a
sequence or cluster of three consonants: |
|
|
|
Examples: |
|
899 1.
castle, wrestling, restless, exactly |
|
2.
soundless, windless, kindness, proudly |
|
3.
three months, the earth’s surface |
|
|
|
|
Deletion of word-final /t/ or /d/ in clusters of
two at a word boundary when the following word begins with a consonant. |
|
|
|
Examples: |
|
8100 1.
This is the best season. |
|
2.
a blind man |
|
3.
They gave me some cold food. |
|
|
|
|
Loss of an unstressed medial vowel (also
referred to as syncope), where the unstressed vowel /«/ or /I/
optionally drops out in some multisyllabic words following the strongly
stressed syllable: |
|
Examples: |
|
8101 |
|
1. “i” privilege, family, aspirin |
|
2. “e” temperature, every, evening, camera, |
|
3. “a” comparable, restaurant |
|
4. “o” potato, chocolate, history, laboratory |
|
5. correct, garage, parade |
|
|
|
|
Sometimes, especially in child’s language,
aphesis occurs, which means loss of an unstressed initial syllable in
highly informal speech. |
|
|
|
8102 |
|
1.
’cause ’cause he’s not here. |
|
2.
’bout ’bout ten o’clock. |
|
3.
’round See you ’round. |
|
|
|
|
Loss of the first noninitial /r/ in a word that
has another /r/ in a following syllable: |
|
|
|
Examples: |
|
8103 |
|
1.
February, governor, surprise |
|
|
|
In some dialects, |
|
2. library, temperature |
|
|
|
|
Loss of final /v/ in “of” (i.e., reduction to
schwa) before words with initial consonants: |
|
|
|
Examples: |
|
8104 |
|
lots of trouble |
|
waste of time |
|
short of money |
|
|
|
|
Loss of initial /h/ and /D/ in
pronominal forms in connected speech: |
|
|
|
Examples: |
|
8105 |
|
1.
ask him |
|
2.
Did he |
|
3.
tell them |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
8106 |
|
1. does he: What does he do? |
|
2. did he: Where did he go? |
|
3. has he: Has he done anything yet? |
|
4. is he: Who is he? |
|
|
|
|
The insertion of a sound into the middle of a
word is known as Epenthesis. For instance, something is pronounced as [ÈsÃmpTIN] rather than [ÈsÃmTIN] and /p/ is added between /m/ and
/T/. In inflations of regular plural and past endings, epenthesis will
occur if /s/ is added to sibilants and /d/ is added to alveolar stops. |
|
Epenthesis can be very common for Mandarin
speakers when consonant clusters occur within a word or in the linking of
words. |
|
For examples, it would be difficult for
Mandarin speakers to say “clear” as [klIr]. [k«lIr] is often heard because consonant cluster is
rare in Mandarin as well as other dialects spoken in China and Taiwan. |
|
|
|
|
8107 |
|
1. rose roses [roz] [roz®z] |
|
2. watch watches [wAtS] [wAtS®z] |
|
3. judge judges [dZÃdZ] [dZÃdZ®z] |
|
4. buzz buzzes [bÃz] [bÃz®z] |
|
8108 |
|
5. fade faded [fed] [fed®d] |
|
6. add added [Qd] [Qd®d] |
|
7. hate hated [het] [het®d] |
|
8. heat heated [hit] [hit®d] |
|